Notes on the Affective Turn

Though affect has often been conflated with emotion, affect, at least according to Brian Massumi is not emotion. But Stenner, drawing on Spinoza, does not make such a sharp distinction.

In the growing literature of the ‘affective turn’, affect is often sharply distinguished from emotion and comes to include issues of mimesis, suggestibility, imitation, contagion and so forth that feed into the wishes, desires, aspirations and other asubjective vectors of becoming that cannot easily be reduced to the self-contained ‘subject’ beloved of liberal humanism, or to existing structural constraints.

Stenner, P. and Moreno, Eduardo (2013). Liminality and affectivity: the case of deceased organ donation. Subjectivity, 6(3) 229–253. doi:10.1057/sub.2013.9.

I agree with Stenner that affect and emotion may be used synonymously. But according to Massumi, affect, also known as intensity, is “always prior to and/or outside of conscious awareness” (Massumi, 2002), whereas emotions, in systems-theoretical terms, are consciously observable. Affect can be detected through a skin conductance response prior to the conscious registration of emotion.

If we can to make a distinction, we can call emotion affect translated into meaningful experience; it might also be an experience that can be placed on the plot line of a narrative or understood from the perspective of linear causality. Emotions are produced through self-observation, but affect is (or may be) an autonomic nervous system response to something in the environment, and it occurs prior to or outside psychic-system self-observation. Affect occurs too quickly to be placed on a narrative plot line; it happens outside of causal relations. It doesn’t  happen in the temporal dimension, which means it’s an event, not a structure.

In systems theoretical-terms, human experience is embedded within organic, psychic, and social systems. Psychic and social systems can be temporalized, distinguished as before/after. Thus if affect is “always prior to and/or outside of conscious awareness” (Massumi, 2002), this means that affect is not observed by the psychic system. That is to say, affect is not produced by the psychic system’s self-observation; it is not filtered by the psychic system’s distinctions (e.g., before/after), which include the difference between past and present. Affect can only be observed “from the outside” as translated into emotion.

Emotions, but not affects, are observed by a psychic system, and often social systems as well. An organic, physiological body is perturbed by something in its environment, such as the sight of an overcast sky and the perception of cold air, and then consciousness might experience (observe) the emotion of sadness. Or an expectation might be disappointed, resulting in sadness. This is more or leIn either case, the emotion is a selection, a reduction of affective complexity. In other words, the emotion is constructed by an observing system. Affect is too complex to be observed by psychic or social systems. The complexity must be reduced through selection. Emotions, then, are contingent rather than necessary.

For present purposes, intensity will be equated with affect. . . .  Affect is most often used loosely as  a synonym for emotion. But . . . emotion and affect–if affect is intensity–follow different logics and pertain to different orders. An emotion is a subjective content, the sociolinguistic fixing of the quality of an experience which is from that point onward defined as personal. Emotion is qualified intensity, the conventional, consensual point of insertion of intensity into semantically and semiotically formed progressions, into narrativizable action-reaction circuits, into function and meaning. It is intensity owned and recognized. It is crucial to theorize the difference between affect and emotion. If some have the impression that affect has waned, it is because affect is unqualified. As such, it is not ownable or recognizable and is thus resistant to critique.  (Massumi (2002). Parables of the Virtual, 27-28)

 

[The] body is radically open, absorbing impulses quicker than they can be perceived. . .

Brain waves of healthy volunteers were monitored by electroencephalograph (EEG) machine. The subjects were asked to flex a finger at a moment of their choosing and to recall the time of their decision by noting the spatial clock position of a revolving dot. The flexes came 0.2 seconds after they clocked the decision, but the EEG machine registered significant brain activity 0.3 seconds before the decision. . . .

Asked to speculate on what implications all this might have for the doctrine of free will, the researcher, Benjamin Libet, proposes that “we may exert free will not by initiating intentions but by vetoing, acceding, or otherwise responding to them after they arise.”

In other words, the half second is missed not because it is empty but  because it is overfull, in excess of the actually performed action and of its ascribed meaning. Will and consciousness are subtractive. They are limitative, derived functions that reduce a complexity too rich to be functionally expressed. (29)

This “vetoing, acceding, or otherwise responding to [intentions] after they arise” would be a selection–a selection from a surplus of possibilities, or perhaps a virtual surplus.

Intensity is incipience, incipient action and expression. Intensity is not only incipience. It is also the beginning of a selection: the incipience of mutually exclusive pathways of action and expression, all but one of which will be inhibited, prevented from actualizing themselves completely. . . .

Something that happens too quickly to have happened, actually, is virtual. The body is as immediately virtual as it is actual. The virtual, the pressing crowd of incipiences and tendencies, is a realm of potential. (30)

Massumi traces these ideas through Spinoza, Bergson, and Deleuze.

We can describe affect as a virtual/actual two-sided form.

The autonomy of affect is its participation in the virtual. Its autonomy is its openness. . . . If there were no escape, no excess or remainder, no fade-out to infinity, the universe would be without potential, pure entropy, death. Actually existing, structured things live in and through that which escapes them. (35)

Perceptions and emotions are ways of capturing affect.

Advertisements
This entry was posted in Uncategorized. Bookmark the permalink.

4 Responses to Notes on the Affective Turn

  1. Pingback: Affect & Resonanzfähigkeit | Autopoiesis: Producing and Reproducing Systems Theory

  2. Pingback: Brian Massumi on threat and the autopoiesis of affective logic | Autopoiesis: Producing and Reproducing Systems Theory

  3. Pingback: Reusable Communication Media, Political Legitimacy, etc. | Autopoiesis: Producing and Reproducing Systems Theory

  4. Pingback: Paul Stenner on Emotion and Luhmann | Autopoiesis: Producing and Reproducing Systems Theory

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out / Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out / Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out / Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out / Change )

Connecting to %s